Mixed-species flocks: an example of niche construction?
نویسندگان
چکیده
Mixed-species flocks result from co-evolved relationships between participants: interspecific gregariousness that varies in strength in different species pairs or groups. Such inter-specific associations of birds are a characteristic of most avian communities, observed in diverse habitats from forests to grasslands, estuaries to the open ocean, worldwide in both high latitudes and in the tropics. We explore the consequences of mixed-species flocks in shaping the selective environment and discuss whether the participants in flocks should be identified as niche constructors. As a result of the decision to associate with other species, a bird alters its relationship with competitors; potentially gains access to resources; becomes safer from predators; and may change microhabitat use. The recurrent patterns in the behaviour of disparate unrelated species active in mixed-species flocks have led investigators to conclude that similar selective pressures have shaped their behaviour. However mixed-species flocks are variable in their characteristics, the birds active in them are diverse in form and behaviour, and the selective pressures that have shaped their propensity to join mixed-species flocks must be various. In forming mixed-species flocks, species with specialised roles at the centre of flocks are creating a complex social environment that represents an important biotic selective force shaping their own ecology and behaviour, and that of other species within the avian community. In this review we consider how the spectrum of inter-specific relationships in mixed-species flocks make them an interesting focus for further studies of niche construction by relocation. INTRODUCTION Inter-specific competition and niche differentiation have been a central focus in ecology and the influence of species upon one another recognised as important in shaping avian communities (Cody 1985; Wiens 1989). Now the evolutionary processes driving niche differentiation are being re-evaluated in a new theoretical context, niche construction, the process by which organisms drive environmental change, modify their relative niches, and as a result become exposed to different selective pressures (Laland et al. 2004; Laland & Sterelny 2006; Krakauer et al. 2009). Mixed-species flocks of birds, or of birds with other vertebrates, are ubiquitous, observed in diverse habitats from forests to grasslands, estuaries to the open ocean, worldwide in both high latitudes and in the tropics. They are particularly characteristic of forest bird communities, and have been studied extensively at temperate latitudes (e.g. Morse 1970, 1978; Carrascal & Moreno 1992; Krams 1996) and in the tropics (e.g. Davis 1946; Buskirk 1976; Gradwohl & Greenberg 1980; Bell 1982; Munn 1985; Hino 1998; Thiollay & Jullien 1998). Mixedspecies flocks are particularly noted as an important feature of tropical forests, where at times the majority of birds observed are in such flocks rather than apart from them (Latta & Wunderle 1996; King & Rappole 2001). Are mixed-species flocks an example of niche construction? Niche construction is more commonly associated with environmental engineers, creatures such as beavers or ants that modify their physical environment and transform the selective regime for future generations (Odling-Smee et al. 1996; Wright et al. 2002). These species have adaptations that give them an advantage in the habitat of their own construction; their offspring have greater success because of a genetic and an ecological inheritance. However, it has been argued (and disputed) that niche construction not only results from environmental engineering (‘perturbation’), but also from ‘relocation’ (Odling-Smee et al. 2003; Laland & Sterelny 2006). Individuals expose themselves to novel selection pressures as the result of their decisions, for example to occupy a given habitat, in the process influencing selection on other species and driving co-evolutionary episodes (Laland & Boogert 2008). In forming inter-specific associations, species with specialised roles within mixed-species flocks are creating a complex social environment that represents an important biotic selective force shaping their own ecology and behaviour, and that of other species. A large literature on mixed-species foraging flocks of birds has been accumulating for more than a century (see Bates 1863 and the comprehensive review by Rand 1954), including elegant detailed behavioural studies (Moynihan 1962; Munn 1985). This represents one category of social aggregation among many (e.g. roosting, migration, breeding), but this particular category – related to the location and capture of food – is peculiar in the recurrence of inter-specific associations and the strength of some alliances. Here we review the literature, considering the evolution of flocking behaviour and the spectrum of co-evolved inter-specific affiliation – from diffusely co-evolved species groups to highly specialised obligate associates. We explore the vocabulary that has been used to describe the roles of species within mixed-species flocks, as these terms reflect the spectrum of inter-dependency and likely trade-offs in the evolution of flocking behaviour. We suggest that mixed-species flocks represent a good system for the study of niche construction, and the following review aims to direct attention to areas meriting investigation in this fresh light. WHAT IS A MIXED-SPECIES FORAGING FLOCK? Mixed-species foraging flocks of birds are associations in which participants seek each other out, actively initiating and maintaining their association; the active interactions of a core set of species at the centre of mixed-species flocks differentiate them from feeding aggregations in which birds simply converge on food, water, confined patches of habitat, or a safe roost (Morse 1970). Mixed-species foraging flocks emerge as a result of the co-evolved relationships between participants that vary in strength between different species pairs or groups (Rand 1954; Moynihan 1962). Birds converging on food such as at a fruiting tree may be competing for limited resources, modifying selection, however the inter-specific associations are often ephemeral (Rand 1954) and the impact fleeting. In practice mixed-species foraging flocks have been defined loosely by the habitat or microhabitat where they occur, the foods taken by participants (e.g. krill, insects), or the species predictably at their centre (e.g. Caldwell 1981; Hoffman et al. 1981; Munn 1985; Remsen 1985; Hunt et al. 1988; King & Rappole 2001). In forests, criteria for differentiating mixed-species flocks of birds from aggregations may include evidence that participants are moving together – some definitions point to how fast birds are moving and how far apart they are – or calls and signals may provide evidence that birds are affiliated (Wiley 1980; Hutto 1987; Gram 1998; Maldonado-Coelho & Marini 2003). Forest flocks are often conspicuous with incessant calling and signalling (posturing, wing fluttering), thought to be important for initiating and maintaining mixed-species associations (Moynihan 1962; Goodale & Kotagama 2005a). The size of mixed-species flocks is extremely variable and not useful in distinguishing them from aggregations. In all examples of mixed-species flocks of passerines there is a small highly interactive core of a few species, which are joined by more, sometimes a great many more, transient participants (Munn 1985). The ‘bird waves’ of the tropical forests can number 100 or more individuals (Rand 1954; Diamond 1987), however most forest flocks are relatively small (Table 1). Studies comparing flocks in various habitats and in different seasons indicate that more species join flocks when resources are scarce or dispersed (Morse 1970; Croxall 1976; Gram 1998; Maldonado-Coelho & Marini 2003); mixed-species foraging flocks are typical in winter in high latitude wooded habitats, and usually in the dry season in the tropics (Morse 1970; Croxall 1976; Earlé 1983; Poulsen 1996; Develey & Peres 2000). Larger numbers of species participate in the flocks observed in late summer and autumn in high latitudes, principally as the result of the addition of a diversity of migrant species (Morse 1970; Rodewald & Brittingham 2002; Hobson & van Wilgenburg 2006). Experimental evidence supports the proposal that resource distribution is important in shaping flocking behaviour (Berner & Grubb 1985; Kubota & Nakamura 2000). Table 1. Mean number of individuals and participating species in various mixed-species flocks in forests Location of study (number of flocks observed) Mean number of individuals Mean number of participating species Total number of participating species Source Brazil, rainy season (42) 9.1 5.8 58 Maldonado-Coelho & Marini 2003 Brazil, dry season (50) 10.9 6.0 71 Maldonado-Coelho & Marini 2003 Mexico, tropical deciduous forest (23) 10.6 6.5 27 Gram 1998 Mexico, dry pine/oak forest (42) 25.9 9.9 38 Gram 1998 Mexico, cloud forest (24) 19.3 9.3 39 Gram 1998 Hispaniola (180) 11.3 7.1 46 Latta & Wunderle 1996 Indonesia (20) 18.6 11.3 36 Croxall 1976 Burma (73) 19.3 6.8 52 King & Rappole 2001 Bolivia, Andes (63) 5.4 2.8 10 Herzog et al. 2002 Bolivia, Andes (40) 5.7 3.5 Moynihan 1962 Ecuador, Andes (89) 5.4 3.4 Moynihan 1962 Columbia, Andes (34) 5.8 3.9 Moynihan 1962 Louisiana (52) 12.1 14 Morse 1970 Maryland, coniferous forest (106) 12.8 14 Morse 1970 Maryland, mixed forest (60) 17.9 13 Morse 1970 Maine, mixed forest, summer (35) 16.4 23 Morse 1970 Saskatchuan, coniferous forest, summer (215) 41.1 6.6 67 Hobson & van Wilgenburg 2006 Pennsylvania, deciduous forest, autumn (220) 22.1 9.3 60 Rodewald & Brittingham 2002 The duration and strength of the relationships between species at the centre of flocks varies from transient to long-term stable associations, and has been identified by Munn & Terborgh (1979) as the basis of ranking flocks on a continuum. They suggest seabirds, forming casual feeding associations, should sit at one end of the continuum, and at the other end lowland Neotropical antbird flocks, which form permanent, life-long associations cooperatively defending a common territory, represent the most advanced form of the phenomenon. But the size and stability of flock membership sometimes varies enormously and numbers of birds participating fluctuate over short time frames (Powell 1979; Terborgh 1990; Thiollay & Jullien 1998). In the mixed-species flocks of the Neotropics there are frequently no more than one or two individuals of each species (Davis 1946; Alves & Cavalcanti 1996; Jullien & Clobert 2000); any increase in flock size is usually the result of more species becoming involved, not more individuals of any one species joining (Greig-Smith 1978a; Powell 1979). In the Andes species assemblages vary markedly across the habitat mosaic shaped by altitude and aspect, with parallel changes in the species composition of mixed-species flocks, and less stability in the interspecific relationships (Moynihan 1962). In the marine environment seabird flocks have been described as temporary feeding associations (Munn & Terborgh 1979), however this does not correctly identify the strength of the relationships between some species. There exist simple transient foraging flocks of seabirds, but also mixed-species foraging flocks in which the species at the core of the flocks show strong inter-specific affinities (Murphy 1936; Sealy 1973; Hoffman et al. 1981; Harrison et al. 1991). In the Southern Ocean the tube-nosed seabirds (Procellariiformes) occur more often in mixed-species associations than apart from them (Murphy 1936). There exist poorly understood inter-dependencies that reflect differing flight dynamics, diving abilities and sensory capacity. Differences exist between species in their ability to find prey either directly (e.g. olfactory capability across different spatial scales; Nevitt 2000), or indirectly (e.g. observation of other predators; Harrison et al 1991). For example, black-browed albatrosses (Thalassarche melanophrys) are unambiguous leaders in mixed-species flocks feeding on Antarctic krill (Euphausia superba) around South Georgia (Harrison et al. 1991); interactions with a small group of other species suggesting co-evolved species affiliations. In this case the black-browed albatrosses appear to be better able to track the foraging activities of fur seals (Arctocephalus gazella) and macaroni penguins (Eudyptes chrysolophus) which drive krill to the surface. Seabirds have varied adaptive inter-specific relationships with other seabird species, with predatory fish such as tuna, with seals and with cetaceans (Au & Pitman 1986; Harrison et al. 1991; Pitman & Ballance 1992). Mixed-species seabird flocks vary in complexity and the spectrum of inter-specific relationships represented has been understated in the literature. The function of the mixed-species flocks will largely determine the nature of the interaction between participants and the extent to which stable associations are formed. Whereas seabirds are generally not at risk from predators when at sea and the function of mixed-species flocks relates to foraging (Rand 1954; Hoffman et al. 1981), terrestrial birds are highly vulnerable to predators and many species of open habitats in particular form mixed-species flocks. Herons, egrets and waders appear to have a strong affinity for one another and coalesce in mixed-species flocks (Nichols 1931; Caldwell 1981). Passerines of open habitat are also vulnerable to predation, and mixed-species flocks of finches and other granivorous birds are commonly observed (Marler 1956; Cody 1971; Rubenstein et al. 1977; Canales-Delgadillo et al. 2008). Members of mixed-species flocks experience a different balance of selective pressures, and the proximity of competitors is likely to shape foraging behaviour. Barnard & Thompson (1985) explored the consequences of mixed-species associations in a study of lapwings (Vanellus vanellus), golden plovers (Pluvialis apricaria) and black-headed gulls (Larus ridibundus) an example of a co-evolved inter-specific association. Mixed-species flocks are seen across taxa and in most environments and not surprisingly there is enormous variation in the inter-specific relationships between participants. Understanding the extent to which mixed-species flocks structure avian communities (Greenberg & Gradwohl 1986; Powell 1989; Mönkkönen 1996; Willson 2004) requires differentiating highly co-evolved inter-dependent species from diffusely co-evolved species. Many species associations are not treated as mixed-species flocks, such as flocks of ducks in which congeners show clear affinity (Johnsgard 1978). Foraging flocks of waterfowl are not discussed with the same language used in the description of mixed-species foraging flocks, despite the fact there exist predictable foraging associations such as that between gadwalls (Anas strepera) and American coot (Fulica americana) (McKnight & Hemp 1998). Rand (1954) terms the foraging behaviour common in inter-specific associations of ducks as ‘communal pilfering’ – which he describes as a ‘more or less peaceful appropriation of food.’ But there are many similarities between this and the associations described as mixed-species flocks; in both cases the flocks include scroungers, and the benefits are unequally distributed between participants. However the literature on forest flocks tends to assume a simple model with convergence evident worldwide in which there are strong mutually beneficial interspecific affinities and a high degree of inter-dependence. While mixed-species flocks are present in all temperate and tropical forests and are convincingly important in these avian communities, but not all represent the apogee of the phenomenon. CHARACTERISING ROLES OF FLOCK PARTICIPANTS The degree of inter-dependence of forest birds in mixed-species flocks has been represented in the vocabulary used in describing the different roles of species (Winterbottom 1949; Munn & Terborgh 1979). From early observations some species were readily identified as ‘regulars’ in flocks (Davis 1946), or as critical to the formation of the flock or ‘primary association formers’ (Gannon 1934). ‘Nuclear species’ and ‘core species’ are the terms used most consistently to identify the species that have this central role in flocks (Winterbottom 1949; Rand 1954; Hutto 1994; Hino 1998). Hoffman et al. (1981) used the term ‘catalyst’ instead to identify the seabird species that through their behaviour attracted other species and so initiated mixed-species foraging flocks. In fact there are different types of leader or nuclear species (Moynihan 1962; Goodale & Kotagama 2005b), and a great many marginal species with varying propensity to participate in flocks (Munn & Terborgh 1979; King & Rappole 2001). In many flocks more than one nuclear species is present and they may differ in their behaviour; Moynihan (1962) identified ‘active or passive’ nuclear species – and in many studies a number of species are central to flock formation and maintenance but one acts as a leader and another more of a follower. In the Central American ‘blue and green tanager and honeycreeper alliance’ described by Moynihan (1962), plain-coloured tanagers (Tangara inornata) are identified as passive nuclear species in that they are joined and followed by individuals of other species, but have little tendency themselves to join and follow other species. Green honeycreepers (Chlorophanes spiza) are identified as active nuclear species, because they both join and follow other species and also attract followers. This distinction is repeated in other mixed-species foraging flocks of tropical forests such as those described in New Guinea (Diamond 1987) and Madagascar (Hino 1998). The different types of nuclear species may relate to their different roles in flocks, some intra-specifically gregarious leaders (Hutto 1994; Sridhar et al. 2009), while others are not particularly gregarious but good sentinels (Goodale & Kotagama 2005b). There have doubtless been inconsistencies in the use of this vocabulary, a problem discussed by Winterbottom (1949), and a problem exacerbated by the fact that some species vary in their roles in flocks in different parts of their range (Moynihan 1979). Identifying the role of a given species will depend upon the particular avifauna and the evolved relationships. Many of the definitions used for tropical forests are inappropriate for characterising birds active in mixed-species flocks of temperate forests (Farley et al. 2008), and vice versa (Stutchbury & Morton 2001). Hence, definitions are often specific to an avian community, or even a given study site. For example, Gram (1998) working in Mexico, identified nuclear species as ‘a species that was intraspecifically gregarious (mean number per flock > 3 individuals; Winterbottom 1943, Greig-Smith 1978a), was a regular flock participant (present in more than 50% of flocks; McClure 1967), was followed more than it followed others (Munn & Terborgh 1979), displayed a conspicuous plumage or behaviour (Chipley 1977), and remained with the flock continuously.’ This array of characteristics is common to many nuclear species in passerine flocks (Hutto 1994; Goodale & Kotagama 2005a). Various other types of flock members have been described: ‘circumference species’ (Winterbottom 1949; Powell 1985), ‘peripheral species’ (Sullivan 1985; Hoffman et al. 1981), ‘joining species’ (Munn 1985), ‘occasional species’ (Munn & Terborgh 1979; Farley et al. 2008), ‘attendant species’ (Rand 1954; Moynihan 1962; Powell 1985; Maldonado-Coelho & Marini 2003) ‘satellite species’ (Dolby & Grubb 1998; Goodale & Kotagama 2005b) and ‘accidental species’ (Davis 1946; Winterbottom 1949; Jullien & Clobert 2000; Farley et al. 2008). Munn & Terborgh (1979) identified different types of ‘joining species’ in their study of lowland Neotropical flocks, for example territorial birds that joined flocks as they passed through their territory, or species from canopy flocks that occasionally switched to understory flocks. Other participants move in and out of flocks, sometimes over short time frames, with varying propensity to participate depending on season, time of day, climate and the species composition of the flock (Powell 1979). Hutto (1994) found that the probability of observing attendant species in mixed-species flocks of small insectivores in Mexico was predicted by their relative abundance in the avian community. The birds at the periphery of mixed-species flocks range from species that are inter-specifically gregarious and usually in flocks, to a great many more opportunists. Thus there exists a spectrum of followers in mixed-species associations from highly co-evolved species dependent upon flocks to diffusely coevolved attendants. The loss of the nuclear species changes the propensity for other species to coalesce into flocks. Diamond (1987) documented the geographic variation over the archipelago of islands around Papua New Guinea, and found that flocks were often altogether absent from particular islands those without the nuclear species acting as catalysts to flock formation. Maldonado-Coelho & Marini (2003) found that the loss of the nuclear species Habia rubica in fragmented habitat in coastal Brazil resulted in the absence of lowland mixed-species flocks, despite the presence of most of the other species typical of the flocks. In these examples, the social cohesion of birds associated in mixed-species flocks depended on the activity of one or a few members of diverse avian communities. The opportunity to join mixed-species flocks appears to be important for a diversity of species; local extinction of catalyst or nuclear species may have a disproportionate impact and has been identified as a potentially important conservation issue in both marine and terrestrial ecosystems (e.g. Hoffman et al. 1981; Goodale and Kotagama 2005b). PREDATOR AVOIDANCE OR FEEDING EFFICIENCY? There is no doubt that a bird may be less vulnerable to predators as the result of participating in a mixed-species flock. The birds benefit from ‘safety in numbers’, and also from the ‘many eyes effect’ – the vigilance and alarm calls of other flock participants (Hamilton 1971; Pulliam 1973; Elgar 1989; Terborgh 1990; Roberts 1996; Beauchamp 2003). The potential advantage for a species in forming inter-specific groups rather than flocking with conspecifics may be in the association with other species that have greater sensory acuity, use a different microhabitat or otherwise behave in such a way that they are more likely to detect predators. The most cited potential foraging benefit of participation in mixed-species flocks is the increased foraging efficiency as the result of shared vigilance (Wiley 1980; Powell 1985; Metcalfe 1989; Roberts 1996; Sasvári & Hegyi 1998; Thiollay 1999; Herzog et al. 2002; Sridhar et al 2009). However, foraging benefits and predator evasion are impossible to regard independently (Morse 1978; Hutto 1994), and it is perhaps unnecessary to do so. In examining the present functional significance of social foraging, security and energetic benefits are not mutually exclusive (Giraldeau & Caraco 2000). The balance of costs and benefits will vary enormously for species with differing feeding behaviours; there exist foraging strategies, for instance bark gleaning or exploration of epiphytes that may make birds particularly vulnerable. Diamond (1987) observed that birds-of-paradise are not able to watch for predators while probing epiphytes, and this ‘close focus’ feeding behaviour makes them particularly vulnerable. The benefits of associating in a mixed-species flock would be more than that expected by the dilution effect, because species differ in their sensory capacity to detect predators early. Some species have clear functional roles as sentinels (Powell 1985; Munn 1986; Jullien & Thiollay 1998). A recurrent pattern is the association of a diversity of small birds with vigilant flock members, notably sallying species such as drongos (Dicrurus) and flycatchers (Terborgh 1990; Goodale & Kotagama 2005a). An alternative is in the association with a numerous vocal species; an abundance of companions helps ensure the detection of danger (Goodale & Kotagama 2005a). There is evidence that woodpeckers and nuthatches seek out parid species as an anti-predator strategy (Sullivan 1985; Dolby & Grubb 1998, 2000). In an experiment involving playback of chickadee vocalisations, Sullivan (1985) found that downy woodpeckers (Picoides pubescens) reduced their level of vigilance when they could hear chickadee calls, a repeated indication of the presence of their vigilant flock mates. Some members of flocks benefit less than others because of the social dynamics within the flock; studies of European parid flocks have demonstrated a cost of subordination, in which young and other subordinate birds in flocks are forced to move to peripheral perches where they become more vulnerable to predation (Ekman & Askemno 1984; Ekman 1987; Suhonen 1993; Suhonen et al. 1993; Krams 1996; Krams 1998). However even though juvenile flock members are sometimes at greater risk from predators than adults, and spend more time in vigilance both against predators and aggressive con-specifics (Carrascal & Moreno 1992), given their inexperience they still are likely to benefit as the result of feeding with adults, which lead them to good foraging sites (Hogstad 1989). There are a number of proposals for how birds in mixed-species flocks may find food more effectively (Ward & Zahavi 1973; Morse 1978; Galef & Giraldeau 2001). Individuals of different species may benefit from each other through social learning or response facilitation (social facilitation) (Byrne 1994). Area copying and social facilitation have been demonstrated experimentally (Krebs 1973; Sasvári & Hegyi 1998). The success or failure of a bird in a given patch represents public information (Giraldeau & Caraco 2000) and birds seek out sites where flock mates have been successful – e.g. a new branch or tree (Waite & Grubb 1988). Greig-Smith (1978b) found Seychelles sunbirds (Nectarinia dussumiera) foraged more effectively when they followed Seychelles white-eyes (Zosterops modesta). When the sunbirds shadowed the foraging white-eyes they concentrated their activity in trees richer in their insect prey. The whiteeyes appeared to be inherently better equipped to sample the environment and evaluate the relative profitability of patches. In a complex landscape, birds may benefit from the community memory, different members of the flock having had experience in finding food in the past lead the others to profitable patches (Giraldeau & Caraco 2000). If birds learn how to locate more types of food, or learn how to solve foraging problems as the result of associating with other species, they gain access to otherwise unavailable food. Some individuals have more need than others for information on where food can be found, for example migrant birds joining mixed-species flocks at migratory stopovers. Birds also benefit if they form associations with other species differing in their sensory acuity or modes of locomotion, such that food is detected that would otherwise be missed. For instance, seabirds associate with marine mammals that locate prey using echolocation (e.g. Pitman & Ballance 1992). Surface-feeding seabirds form mixedspecies flocks in association with penguins, auks or other diving vertebrates such as cetaceans that locate food and also make it available at the surface (Murphy 1936; Hoffman et al. 1981; Obst & Hunt 1990; Harrison et al. 1991; Grebmeier & Harrison 1992). The ‘gang theory’ has been proposed as an important advantage to participants in inter-specific associations (Diamond 1987). There is a greater opportunity for overwhelming territorial defences by associating with other species (Dunbar 1988), and the benefit of associating with a ‘gang’ may explain the occasional benefit gained by some opportunistic flock participants – territorial birds which become carried along by a flock beyond their own territory boundary. For instance, Moynihan (1962) noted that yellow-thighed finches (Pselliophorus tobialis) would follow mixed-species flocks into the territories of their neighbours. One way in which mixed-species flocks facilitate foraging is the 'beater effect', in which birds benefit from insects flushed by other flock participants (Munn & Terborgh 1979; Powell 1985; Diamond 1987; Rodrigues et al. 1994; Hino 1998). While positive interaction is typical within mixed-species flocks of passerines, kleptoparasites are evident, either pilfering food items from flock mates, or dominating patches located by a flock-mate. Behavioural observations suggest that the relationship of species in these flocks may be long-standing and stable, and the costs may be balanced by benefits accrued from their presence – notably alarm calls. Munn (1986) observed species acting as sentinels in mixed-species flocks, Lanio versicolor and Thamnomanes schistogynus, making false warning calls, and so taking the attention of flock mates off intended prey that they then claimed. Kleptoparasitism in many instances in social foraging groups does not appear to be aggressive but instead tolerated theft (Giraldeau & Caraco 2000; King & Rappole 2001). Indeed aggression between species active in mixed-species flocks is extremely rare and may be evident between conspecifics but not between species, even when neighbouring flocks of passerines meet (Buskirk et al. 1972). Thus there are benefits of inter-specific associations in environments where there is spatial and temporal unpredictability of food, particularly where birds are vulnerable to predators, and this is the likely starting point for the evolution of mixed-species flocks. But many participants in mixed-species flocks appear to be subject to strong selection for co-existence, with wider consequences to their ecology and behaviour. Weighing up the costs and benefits of participation in terms of feeding benefits and security does not account for the new level of complexity created by the social environment of the flock. The cost of competition must have had some historical role in shaping flock composition and behaviour: which species associate, from which feeding guilds, and how far apart do they feed? For many followers and joiners such a balance may still be important in determining the pattern of participation. But presently some of the species persistent in mixed-species flocks may be beyond the point of no return; they have a suite of adaptations for existing within a flock such that the benefits of living in the flock far outweigh the costs.
منابع مشابه
Bird mixed-flocks and nuclear species in a tecoma savanna in the Pantanal.
In bird mixed flocks, a prominent species, the so-called nuclear species, improves the cohesion and maintenance of the flocks, while other less conspicuous species are assumed as satellite. In this study we described the composition, as well as examined the existence of both nuclear and satellite species in mixed flocks of a savanna in the Pantanal. The observations were developed using three t...
متن کاملBirds of Many Feathers: The Formation and Structure of Mixed- Species Flocks of Forest Birds
Considering the differences in the social and ecological requirements of the different participants, the cohesive movement of animal groups is an amazing phenomenon. Associations composed of many species, each with its own intraspecific social organization and species-specific requirements, seem even more improbable. Yet mixed-species fish schools (Ehrlich and Ehrlich 1973; Alevizon 1976), ungi...
متن کاملForaging intention affects whether willow tits call to attract members of mixed-species flocks
Understanding how individual behaviour influences the spatial and temporal distribution of other species is necessary to resolve the complex structure of species assemblages. Mixed-species bird flocks provide an ideal opportunity to investigate this issue, because members of the flocks are involved in a variety of behavioural interactions between species. Willow tits (Poecile montanus) often pr...
متن کاملHistorical and Current Niche Construction in an Anthropogenic Biome: Old Cultural Landscapes in Southern Scandinavia
Conceptual advances in niche construction theory provide new perspectives and a tool-box for studies of human-environment interactions mediating what is termed anthropogenic biomes. This theory is useful also for studies on how anthropogenic biomes are perceived and valued. This paper addresses these topics using an example: “old cultural landscapes” in Scandinavia, i.e., landscapes formed by a...
متن کاملAssembly of avian mixed-species flocks in Amazonia.
Diamond's "assembly rules" model posits that competitive interactions among species govern the composition of avifaunas. Although originally applied to islands in archipelagoes, this controversial set of hypotheses is difficult to test because islands differ in habitat and resource availability, colonization history, and stochastic effects. Permanent mixed-species flocks of Amazonian birds are ...
متن کاملذخیره در منابع من
با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید
عنوان ژورنال:
دوره شماره
صفحات -
تاریخ انتشار 2011